THE PREREQUISITES OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR IN HUMAN ONTOGENY

Authors

  • Irina M. Sozinova Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Russian Federation
  • Alexey A. Sozinov Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences; State Academic University of the Humanities, Russian Federation
  • Seppo J. Laukka University of Oulu, Learning Research Laboratory (LearnLab), Finland
  • Yuri I. Alexandrov Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences; Department of Psychology, National Research University Higher School of Economics; State Academic University of the Humanities, Russian Federation

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.5937/IJCRSEE1701057S

Keywords:

prosocial behavior, in-group, out-group, 3–11-year-old children, moral dilemmas

Abstract

Understanding the development of moral attitudes toward unrelated individuals from different social groups may provide insights into the role of biological and cultural factors in prosocial behavior. Children (3–11 years old, N=80) were presented with moral dilemmas describing a conflict of interests between a con-specific (human) and another species (animals or aliens). Participants were asked to evaluate the behavior of a human in terms of ‘good’ and ‘bad’, and to choose whom they would help: a human aggressor who benefits at the expense of a victim in vital need, or the victim. Results showed that the older children preferred to help non-human victims significantly more often than the younger children. The evaluation of human actions was related to the proportion of such preferences. These findings are discussed from the perspectives of kin selection theory, group selection theory and the system-evolutionary approach. The implications of the study for moral education are suggested.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References

Aleksandrov, YI, & Aleksandrova, NL (2007). Subjective experience and culture. Structure and dynamics. Social Sciences, 38(3), 109-124.

Anokhin, P. K. (1974). Biology and Neurophysiology of the Conditioned Reflex and Its Role in Adaptive Behavior: Scientific and Transl. Ed.-Samuel A. Corson. Transl. Panel-Roland Dartau, Justina Epp, Vladimir Kirilcuk. Pergamon Press.

Arutyunova, KR, Alexandrov, YI, Znakov, VV, & Hauser, MD (2013). Moral judgments in Russian culture: Universality and cultural specificity. Journal of cognition and culture, 13(3-4), 255-285. doi:10.1163/15685373-12342094

Choi, JK, & Bowles S. (2007). The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science, 318(5850), 636-640. doi:10.1126/science.1144237

Cunningham, WA, Johnson, MK, Raye, CL, Gatenby, JC, Gore, JC, & Banaji, MR (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15(12), 806-813. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x

Hamilton, WD (1964). “The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I”. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 7 (1), 1–16. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4

Hamilton, WD (1964). “The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II”. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 7 (1), 17–52. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(64)90039-6

Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, CD, & Singer, T. (2010). Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron, 68(1), 149-160. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003

House, BR, Silk, JB, Henrich, J., Barrett, HC, Scelza, BA, Boyette, AH, ... & Laurence, S. (2013). Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(36), 14586-14591. doi:10.1073/pnas.1221217110

Kinzler, KD, & Spelke, ES (2011). Do infants show social preferences for people differing in race? Cognition, 119(1), 1-9. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.019

Kohlberg, L. (1974). Education, moral development and faith. Journal of Moral Education, 4(1), 5-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0305724740040102

Koonz C. (2003). The Nazi Conscience. Harvard University Press. http://www.faculty.umb.edu/lawrence_blum/courses/290h_09/readings/koonz.pdf

Leimar, O., & Hammerstein, P. (2006). Facing the facts. Journal of evolutionary biology, 19(5), 1403-1405. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01156.x

Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, AF (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322(5898), 58-62. doi:10.1126/science.1158757

Passini, S. (2016). Concern for close or distant others: The distinction between moral identity and moral inclusion. Journal of Moral Education, 45(1), 74-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2016.1156522

Piaget, J. (1997). The moral judgement of the child. Simon and Schuster. http://www.unige.ch/fapse/files/6814/3886/5440/Article_Piaget_jugement-moral_ENG.pdf

Quintana, SM (1998). Children’s developmental understanding of ethnicity and race. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 7(1), 27-45. doi:10.1016/S0962-1849(98)80020-6

Rest, JR, Narvaez, D., Thoma, SJ, & Bebeau, MJ (2000). A neo-Kohlbergian approach to morality research. Journal of moral education, 29(4), 381-395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713679390

Reznikova, Z. (2011). Evolutionary and Behavioural Aspects of Altruism in Animal Communities: Is There Room for Intelligence?. ‘Evolution’Almanac Editors Council: H. Barry III (USA), Yu. Е. Berezkin (Russia), ML Butovs-kaya (Russia), R. Carneiro (USA), Ch. Chase-Dunn (USA), VV Chernykh (Russia), HJM Claessen (Netherlands), D. Christian (Australia), S. Gavrilets (USA), АV Dybo (Russia), K. Yu. Es’ kov (Russia), IV Ilyin, 122. http://www.sociostudies.org/almanac/articles/evolutionary_and_behavioural_aspects_of_altruism_in_animal_communities_is_there_room_for_intelligen/

Rilling, JK, Dagenais, JE, Goldsmith, DR, Glenn, AL, & Pagnoni, G. (2008). Social cognitive neural networks during in-group and out-group interactions. Neuroimage, 41(4), 1447-1461. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.044

Simpson, B., & Willer, R. (2008). Altruism and indirect reciprocity: The interaction of person and situation in prosocial behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 71(1), 37-52. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/019027250807100106

Smetana, J. (1981). Preschool Children’s Conceptions of Moral and Social Rules. Child Development, 52(4), 1333-1336. doi:10.2307/1129527doi:10.2307/1129527

Shvyrkov, VB (1986). Behavioral specialization of neurons and the system-selection hypothesis of learning. Human memory and cognitive capabilities. Amsterdam. Elsevier, 599-611.

Sozinov, AA, Shirinkina, A., Siipo, A., Nopanen, M., Tuominen, T., Laukka, SJ, & Alexandrov, YI (2015). The formation of encouragement achievement and loss avoidance behavior in Finnish and Russian schoolchildren. Voprosy Psihologii 4: 26–37. [In Russian]

Svetlova, M., Nichols, SR, & Brownell, CA (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child development, 81(6), 1814-1827. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x

Vygotski, LS (1929). II. The problem of the cultural development of the child. The pedagogical seminary and journal of genetic psychology, 36(3), 415-434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1929.10532201

West, SA, Griffin, AS, & Gardner, A. (2007a). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17(16), 661-672. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.004

West, SA, Griffin, AS, & Gardner, A. (2007b). Social semantics: altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of evolutionary biology, 20(2), 415-432. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x

Wilson, DS (1975). A theory of group selection. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences, 72(1), 143-146. http://www.pnas.org/content/72/1/143.full.pdf

Wong, R. YM, & Hong, YY (2005). Dynamic influences of culture on cooperation in the prisoner’s dilemma. Psychological science, 16(6), 429-434. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01552.x

Downloads

Published

2017-06-20

How to Cite

M. Sozinova, I. ., A. Sozinov, A. ., J. Laukka, S. ., & I. Alexandrov, Y. . (2017). THE PREREQUISITES OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR IN HUMAN ONTOGENY. International Journal of Cognitive Research in Science, Engineering and Education (IJCRSEE), 5(1), 57–63. https://doi.org/10.5937/IJCRSEE1701057S

Metrics

Plaudit